Video Playback, Affective Witnessing, and the Mobility of Trauma Video Playback, Affective Witnessing, and the Mobility of Trauma: Video Evidence of Violent Crime in the Criminal Justice System Abstract In today’s technologically mediated society, video is increasingly relied upon as an objective and reliable source of evidence in the investigation and prosecution of violent crimes. The now pervasive presence of violent video in the criminal justice system, however, presents new challenges for understanding repeated work-related exposure to and witnessing of potentially traumatic material and its impacts. Thus, this project seeks to qualitatively examine the relational affective processes that occur among criminal justice professionals when violent crimes are captured on video. We present four key categories organized around the circumstances of exposure and its impacts: 1) playback in the investigative and pre-trial process; 2) sharing videos among colleagues; 3) playing videos for victims, witnesses, and families and; 4) transmission in the broader public. Findings suggest this work involves deeply embodied processes where video evidence of violent crime enables a virtual presence at scenes and an emotional proximity to events through new forms of witnessing. These affective experiences are one relational dynamic that keeps witnessing active, thus expanding the mobility of trauma, its reach and potential impacts. Key Words: traumatic exposure, video evidence of violent crime, criminal justice professionals, affective witnessing, mobility of trauma, embodied traumas conviction of violent offenders (Brookman & Jones, 2021; Cook et al., 2021; Westera & Powell, 2017) as well as contributed to the advancement of research and understanding of violent offender behaviours (Lindegaard & Bernasco, 2018; Philpot et al., 2019), its potential to traumatize and re-traumatize viewers has rarely been considered. Nevertheless, the circulation and viewing of traumatic events captured on video, have led to new forms of witnessing (Hjorth & Cumiskey, 2018), and consequently to new forms of workplace-based trauma (Birze, Regehr, et al., 2022b) and victim re-traumatization (Regehr et al., 2021). The now pervasive presence of video evidence throughout the criminal justice system, raises new moral, psychological, ethical, and social concerns, and presents new challenges for the consideration of repeated work- related exposure to and witnessing of potentially traumatic material, its transmission, and the associated impacts. A timely reconceptualization of “witnessing” has centred the relational aspects of witnessing that better account for the affective experiences related to the witnessing and re- witnessing of videoed events among individuals and groups across time and place (Richardson & Schankweiler, 2020). Arising from studies of vicarious witnessing through photographs, conviction of violent offenders (Brookman & Jones, 2021; Cook et al., 2021; Westera & Powell, 2017) as well as contributed to the advancement of research and understanding of violent offender behaviours (Lindegaard & Bernasco, 2018; Philpot et al., 2019), its potential to traumatize and re-traumatize viewers has rarely been considered. Nevertheless, the circulation and viewing of traumatic events captured on video, have led to new forms of witnessing (Hjorth & Cumiskey, 2018), and consequently to new forms of workplace-based trauma (Birze, Regehr, et al., 2022b) and victim re-traumatization (Regehr et al., 2021). The now pervasive presence of video evidence throughout the criminal justice system, raises new moral, psychological, ethical, and social concerns, and presents new challenges for the consideration of repeated work- related exposure to and witnessing of potentially traumatic material, its transmission, and the associated impacts. A timely reconceptualization of “witnessing” has centred the relational aspects of witnessing that better account for the affective experiences related to the witnessing and re- witnessing of videoed events among individuals and groups across time and place (Richardson & Schankweiler, 2020). Arising from studies of vicarious witnessing through photographs, Video Playback, Affective Witnessing, and the Mobility of Trauma Introduction In today’s technologically mediated society, video evidence is increasingly relied upon in the investigation and prosecution of violent crimes. While its ubiquity may have realized new benefits for criminal justice processes such as improved investigative tools, rates of arrest, and survivor stories and artifacts of the Holocaust, the mind reconstructs the experiences of others creating a “powerful experience, whereby people…[live] imaginatively through the experiences of others” (Keats, 2005, p. 171). This initial conceptualization of vicarious witnessing involved experience, including human suffering, and the sharing of these images through visual media creates a new sense of immediacy and presence for a much wider range of viewers (Ellis, 2009; Hjorth & Cumiskey, 2018). Recorded events, according to Ellis (2009), create a sense of “thereness” that has the effect of generating emotional reactions that “feel similar to direct encounters, even as we know them to be distanced and observed encounters” (p. 71). Video capabilities can communicate both experience and affect in a manner that is both intimate and public. This “affective witnessing” is “unbounded by space and time”, that is, it can be newly experienced and re-experienced indefinitely (Hjorth & Cumiskey, 2018, p. 168). Richardson and Schankweiler (2020, p. 242) have recently argued, “Whether an event is inscribed in the bodily memory of the witness or recorded on a smartphone, its registering as an event is necessarily a mediation that enables the potential transmission of the event to others elsewhere.” Accordingly, affective witnessing attends to the moments and relational dynamics that allow for the repetition and endurance of the witnessing experience that “centres encounter, embodiment, affect and intensities of experience” (Richardson & Schankweiler, 2020, p. 237). For instance, when mobile phones are used to document violent experience, including human suffering, and the sharing of these images through visual media creates a new sense of immediacy and presence for a much wider range of viewers (Ellis, 2009; Hjorth & Cumiskey, 2018). Recorded events, according to Ellis (2009), create a sense of “thereness” that has the effect of generating emotional reactions that “feel similar to direct encounters, even as we know them to be distanced and observed encounters” (p. 71). Video capabilities can communicate both experience and affect in a manner that is both intimate and public. This “affective witnessing” is “unbounded by space and time”, that is, it can be newly experienced and re-experienced indefinitely (Hjorth & Cumiskey, 2018, p. 168). Richardson and Schankweiler (2020, p. 242) have recently argued, “Whether an event is inscribed in the bodily memory of the witness or recorded on a smartphone, its registering as an event is necessarily a mediation that enables the potential transmission of the event to others elsewhere.” Accordingly, affective witnessing attends to the moments and relational dynamics that allow for the repetition and endurance of the witnessing experience that “centres encounter, embodiment, affect and intensities of experience” (Richardson & Schankweiler, 2020, p. 237). For instance, when mobile phones are used to document violent Video Playback, Affective Witnessing, and the Mobility of Trauma deliberate acts in which individuals purposively sought to immerse themselves in various sources of information related to a tragic event in an effort to gain deep knowledge of the suffering of distant others. In recent years however, the availability of devices to record human deaths, such as in mass shooting incidents, actual scenes and the associated traumatic experiences of others are brought into view, giving rise to both individual and collective affective witnessing, often blurring the distinction between survivors and vicarious witnesses (Cumiskey & Hjorth, 2019; McGrane et al., 2021). Further, as the viewer is connected to the Video Playback, Affective Witnessing, and the Mobility of Trauma emotions of the sufferer, this painful knowledge acquired through witnessing and through being virtually present at the scene of the suffering, implies a kind of participation on the part of the viewer, and a sense of responsibility to act on the suffering (Kyriakidou, 2015). That is, to witness an event means becoming “responsible to it” (Peters, 2001, p. 708). This adds to the potential of witnessing to become a life changing experience. In a parallel development, recent efforts to reconceptualize trauma have also brought to the fore the relational aspects of traumatization that enable its transmission or travel among and between individuals, what has been described as “trauma contagion” (Coddington, 2017; Coddington & Micieli-Voutsinas, 2017). When working to conceptualize the transmissibility of traumatic incidents from person to person, examining the travel and impact of video evidence of violent crimes provides rich material for analysis. As Hjorth and Cumiskey (2018, p. 175) suggest, video evidence of violent crime is a “vehicle for understanding the textures of tragedy, trauma and horror through affective witnessing”, suggesting new implications for the collection, storage, viewing, and sharing of this content in the workplace and beyond. Such affective witnessing through mobile media and other sources of video evidence brings into sharp relief the potential for unique forms of trauma realized through new forms of witnessing trauma (seeing and hearing events from another place and time). For instance, in the courtroom, such videos may be conceptualized as non-human witnesses and are often deployed precisely because of their propensity to evoke intense emotional responses and manipulate the emotional dynamics among viewers, the courtroom actors (Bens, 2020). Given potentially traumatic video evidence of this sort is increasingly mobilized as an aspect of the day-to-day mechanics of the criminal justice system (Kimpel, 2021), an approach that considers Thus, in this paper we qualitatively examine the mobility of trauma as an emergent relational affective process that occurs among criminal justice professionals, victims, witness, and families, and the broader public, when violent crimes are captured on video. Methods This research adopted a discovery-oriented qualitative design, using constructivist grounded theory method. Originally developed by Glaser and Strauss (Glaser & Strauss, 1967), grounded theory involves an iterative process of data collection and analysis that helps researchers to systematically interrogate their data and generate new concepts that work to foster the growth of theory in understudied areas (Charmaz, 2008; Charmaz & Thornberg, 2021). Further developing their earlier work, Charmaz (2000; 2014) proposed an adaptation – constructivist grounded theory (CGT) – which underpins a grounded theory approach with a constructivist paradigm. That is, CGT asserts that data and meaning, rather than emerging as an objective truth, are co-constructed through the relationship between the researchers and the participants (Birks & Mills, 2015). Thus, in this paper we qualitatively examine the mobility of trauma as an emergent relational affective process that occurs among criminal justice professionals, victims, witness, and families, and the broader public, when violent crimes are captured on video. Methods This research adopted a discovery-oriented qualitative design, using constructivist grounded theory method. Originally developed by Glaser and Strauss (Glaser & Strauss, 1967), grounded theory involves an iterative process of data collection and analysis that helps researchers to systematically interrogate their data and generate new concepts that work to foster the growth of theory in understudied areas (Charmaz, 2008; Charmaz & Thornberg, 2021). Further developing their earlier work, Charmaz (2000; 2014) proposed an adaptation – constructivist grounded theory (CGT) – which underpins a grounded theory approach with a constructivist paradigm. That is, CGT asserts that data and meaning, rather than emerging as an objective truth, are co-constructed through the relationship between the researchers and the participants (Birks & Mills, 2015). Video Playback, Affective Witnessing, and the Mobility of Trauma these encounters and the embodied traumas of this work is warranted (Birze, Regehr, et al., 2022b). Importantly, almost no research has specifically addressed what might now be considered an avalanche of potentially traumatic, obscene and gruesome video evidence of violent crimes for many criminal justice actors (Kimpel, 2021). Furthermore, no research to the best of our knowledge has considered the new mobility of mediated trauma in this context. In support of maximal variation for information-rich sampling, this research engaged in dialogues with police officers, civilian crime analysts, legal professionals (e.g. court reporters, lawyers and judges), and forensic mental health professionals, using the long-interview method Video Playback, Affective Witnessing, and the Mobility of Trauma of data collection (McCracken, 1988) for thick description and credibility (Lietz & Zayas, 2010). The long-interview method uses open-ended questions to foster the exploration of experiences and elicit relevant stories (McCracken, 1988), in this case related to workplace exposure to and use of video evidence of violent crime. Ongoing informed consent for interviews was gained though detailed discussion and review of the consent form with participants. Prior to beginning interviews, one member of the research team explained the purpose of the project, the potential risks involved with discussing the sensitive material and revisiting its traumatic impacts. Participants were reassured that they could determine the pace and length of the interview, break, quit, or withdraw at any time, and that what they shared in the recorded interview would be protected as an anonymous contribution. The proposal was approved by the Human Subjects Research Ethics Board at the University of Toronto. As researchers we remain vigilant to the profoundly emotional nature of our research with the trauma of others, the understanding that we each occupy privileged places of access to the experiences of these professionals, and how this access and research shapes our own emotional responses and outcomes (Drozdzewski & Dominey-Howes, 2015). A psychologically safe and trauma-aware working context and affordance for slow scholarship worked to mitigate the potential impacts. In fact, for each of us, it is because of our positionalities through both personal and professional experiences with potentially traumatic video that we have come to recognize the gravity of this work for ourselves, our families, as well as our participants (Regehr et al., 2019). Further, as researchers, not viewing the videos described in this research has afforded us the privilege of self-protection by maintaining some level of emotional distance from the recorded images. Necessarily, we continually pursue meaningful examination and Video Playback, Affective Witnessing, and the Mobility of Trauma better understanding so that our work might serve as a source of advocacy, informing policy change, not only for those whose professional work places them at greater risk through viewing, but for those who appear in the images as well. Participants Data Analysis Recruited through the extensive professional networks of the authors, as well as subsequent snowball sampling, a purposive yet convenient sample of sixteen criminal justice professionals (11 women and 5 men) participated in interviews ranging from approximately 45 – 120 minutes in length. Inclusion criteria were employment in the criminal justice system and experience engaging with video evidence of violent crimes. Seven participants were members of large urban or national policing organizations, six of whom were detectives, supervising, or senior officers with investigative experience and one of whom was a civilian analyst (coded P###). Eight participants were members of the legal profession including prosecuting attorneys, defense counsel, a law clerk, a court reporter and a judge (coded LP-###). One individual was a forensic mental health professional (coded FMH-###). Years of practice ranged from 4.5 years to over 50, with all participants suggesting that recent technological advances and the accompanying ubiquitous use of recording devices had radically altered the landscape of the criminal justice system with significant increases in exposure to potentially traumatic content. We documented experiences with videoed violence drawing on an affective-discursive psychosocial approach (Wetherell, 2012) to understand affecting episodes, embodied experiences and emergent relational patterns of practice in the transmission of affect (Wetherell, 2013, 2015), in this case, the trauma of others. Through our interviews, we Video Playback, Affective Witnessing, and the Mobility of Trauma captured salient affective processes of viewing, listening, and sharing videoed violence. For instance, given the detail, atmosphere, and intimacy transmitted through the audio-visual (Kyriakidou, 2015), we considered how image, sound, repetition, and transmission are all affective dynamics that can help us understand how video creates an embodied sense of thereness with potentially traumatic ripple effects for professionals, victims and survivors, as well as the general public. The interviews were professionally transcribed verbatim for line-by-line micro-analysis. Open, axial and selective coding as typically used in grounded theory was employed to further determine germane categories (Strauss & Corbin, 1998). Once interviewing and transcribing had begun, constant comparison method was used throughout coding (Strauss & Corbin, 1998). Each author individually coded a portion of the interviews to establish preliminary analytic focuses and categories such as the ubiquity, sources, quality, and importance of video evidence, how it is managed throughout the criminal justice system, the nature and context of viewing, symptoms, effects, and reactions to viewing. We then together examined the coded interviews which both established agreement and provided opportunity to discuss and revise differences. Axial coding was then used to determine the inter-relationships between categories and selective coding built and clarified concepts and helped to integrate and refine the emerging theory according to the perspectives of criminal justice professionals (Birks & Mills, 2015; Corbin & Strauss, 2008). As an iterative and reflexive practice, coding is context dependent, never complete and never reaches a fixed endpoint (Braun & Clarke, 2021). Nevertheless, we aimed to demonstrate saturation and conceptual rigor through our emergent conceptual development and theoretical explanations of the current data (Low, 2019). Video Playback, Affective Witnessing, and the Mobility of Trauma Results Video evidence of violent crime is highly valued by criminal justice professionals due to its perceived objective, reliable, predictable, and compelling nature. As a result, it is often preferred over victim or witness testimony and other more traditional forms of evidence. For instance, participants generally believed – as exemplified by the statement of this judge – that “[victim and witness] perceptions might not be accurate and their memories might not be accurate. Occasionally, they might actually not be telling the truth” (LP-208). Whereas video evidence is perceived as “more real, because it shows you exactly what happened…and it’s certainly different than a witness recounting what happened, because if you’re the trier of fact, whether a juror or judge, you don’t have to sort of picture it in your mind as you’re listening to the witness describe it. You can actually see it with your own eyes...And it’s fixed in time” (LP- 208). One police investigator stated, “video was obviously the best evidence…How can you dispute what you’re actually seeing being recreated?” (P-104). Lawyers concurred that video evidence is now preferred over written statements and witness testimony because it is viewed as a “much less biased or less fallible source of testimony” (LP-206). This lawyer adds “[V]ideo evidence…can be incredibly powerful evidence. Just filling in gaps where a person either because of perception, they just simply couldn’t see something, or because of memory, they might not be able to remember something, it can fill in those gaps.” Further, video evidence is seen to be effective precisely because of its ability to evoke affective witnessing as one lawyer stated: “We rely on it … to sort of put you in the shoes of the participants” (LP-206). Nevertheless, while video evidence may be viewed to serve the interests of justice, participants in this study also noted risks associated with the repeated transmission of these Video Playback, Affective Witnessing, and the Mobility of Trauma “profoundly, emotionally generating” sounds and images (FMH-301) in terms of their potential for trauma contagion or the spread and expansion of trauma from individual to individual across time and place (Coddington, 2017). Categories arising from our discussions with participants are generally organized by circumstances of exposure over time, paying particular attention to the relational aspects of the affecting episodes, including whether they were occasions of individual or collective witnessing, and the associated impacts: playback in the investigative and pre-trial process; sharing videos among colleagues; playing videos for victims, witnesses, and families; and transmission in the broader public. Figure 1 visually represents the mobility and transmission of trauma through the reverberations of each playback. Video is created at the time of the crime, enters the justice system, is viewed and analyzed by criminal justice professionals, is potentially shared with colleagues for various reasons, is reviewed with victims, witnesses and families as part of the justice process, and is eventually broadcast in open court and/or to the broader public. [insert figure 1 about here] Figure 1: The relational dynamics that keep affective witnessing active and how they support the mobility of trauma through repeated exposures in criminal justice processes involving violent video evidence Affective witnessing during investigative and criminal justice processes According to participants, there were unique elements to engaging with videos compared to other forms of evidence, including whether they were viewing the video alone or with others. The close and repeated playback of videos, throughout their mobilization within the criminal justice system, was seen as increasing the risks associated with the work. For Figure 1: The relational dynamics that keep affective witnessing active and how they support the mobility of trauma through repeated exposures in criminal justice processes involving violent video evidence Affective witnessing during investigative and criminal justice processes According to participants, there were unique elements to engaging with videos compared to other forms of evidence, including whether they were viewing the video alone or with others. The close and repeated playback of videos, throughout their mobilization within the criminal justice system, was seen as increasing the risks associated with the work. For Video Playback, Affective Witnessing, and the Mobility of Trauma instance, an officer reflects “You could flip through a dozen pictures really easily and get a sense of what’s going on. But the video, you have to immerse yourself in and sit there and watch it…and you have to be more attentive to viewing it.” (P-104). A senior officer describes the work of those in the forensic video analysis unit who “highlights it, cuts it, pastes 11 Video Playback, Affective Witnessing, and the Mobility of Trauma it, looks at it, dissects it, sees if they can clean it up…they’ll look at it over and over and over again...” and later continues to describe a similar level of engagement with video evidence in other units; “The same with child exploitation, they watch images. I can’t tell you how many times they watch it over and over and over and over again.” (P-101). Regardless of the content and volumes of video evidence, those responsible for its investigative and legal analysis often spoke of intimately engaging with the images and sounds so that the scenes unfolding in front of them might become “second nature”. A senior officer suggests “if you have a shooting and you have two people that walk up to the person and shoot them, then you’re going to watch it over and over and over again to pick out dress, maybe a limp. You’re going to watch it so that it becomes sort of second nature to you,” (P-101). Similarly, an assistant crown attorney notes “the reality of the situation is, to pick up on key details which you need to use at trial, you have to watch it numerous times frame by frame…and in slow motion and intently…then you want to pick up on even maybe the stickers on the shoes…where somebody is getting kicked in the head, you’ve got to look at that frame a number of times and you want it to be really detailed. So, you’re really, really focused in on it.” (LP-205). While an assistant crown attorney suggests that “the first initial reaction is probably the worst one…[because] you know that this image is going to be captured in your brain” (LP-205), an analyst in national security believes the experiences and associated risks shift with each viewing. “There is some risk associated to watching repeated violent videos…something could result that maybe watching something for the tenth time hits you a bit differently.” (P-105). Whereas the “vicarious witnessing” of traumatic events discussed by Keats (2005) is “pieced together from images, stories, and physical artifacts” (p. 175) that encourage (Doughty & Drozdzewski, 2022; Hemsworth, 2016), in addition to repeatedly viewing moving images, participants also spoke of the combination of image and sound as a cumulative element of risk. First, a senior officer suggests of videos that don’t include sound; “we’re almost vicarious, looking at it in a video. It’s kind of fishbowl-ish”, acknowledging the missing information when you cannot hear what is happening inside. This officer then adds “But when you’re listening to somebody and you’re listening to their voice, you assign it to an actual human…[and]…You don’t have to guess at what it sounded like as that person was dying” (P- 101). An investigator, in discussing videoed violence also suggests, “or even hearing it on a phone is far more impactful...because of course it’s the emotion, it’s the words that are used, it’s the state of the victim” (IDP106). A legal professional concurs; “But videos, they’re moving pictures…and voices, and all the dots are connected. When you look at a photo, there’s no voice” and notably adds, “You can’t treat it like every other case that doesn’t have that element in it…I really think our brains just process it so differently” (LP-207). A number of participants suggested that when image and sound were combined, sound is what had the most lasting impacts. For instance, a forensic mental health professional tasked with analysing video of (Doughty & Drozdzewski, 2022; Hemsworth, 2016), in addition to repeatedly viewing moving images, participants also spoke of the combination of image and sound as a cumulative element of risk. First, a senior officer suggests of videos that don’t include sound; “we’re almost vicarious, looking at it in a video. It’s kind of fishbowl-ish”, acknowledging the missing information when you cannot hear what is happening inside. This officer then adds “But when you’re listening to somebody and you’re listening to their voice, you assign it to an actual human…[and]…You don’t have to guess at what it sounded like as that person was dying” (P- 101). An investigator, in discussing videoed violence also suggests, “or even hearing it on a phone is far more impactful...because of course it’s the emotion, it’s the words that are used, it’s the state of the victim” (IDP106). A legal professional concurs; “But videos, they’re moving pictures…and voices, and all the dots are connected. When you look at a photo, there’s no voice” and notably adds, “You can’t treat it like every other case that doesn’t have that element in it…I really think our brains just process it so differently” (LP-207). A number of participants suggested that when image and sound were combined, sound is what had the most lasting impacts. For instance, a forensic mental health professional tasked with analysing video of Video Playback, Affective Witnessing, and the Mobility of Trauma viewers to imaginatively reconstruct the experiences of others, participants in this study spoke of immersing themselves in videoed events as less vicarious, more tenable and embodied “affecting episodes” (Wetherell, 2015). As sound also enables emotional connection with other individuals and contexts repeated sexual assault and torture found; “The worst thing for me was the audio…it was either the despair in their voice and sometimes the audio combined with the emotional distress…that I just couldn’t tolerate” (FMH-301). A law clerk similarly finds, “It was the sounds. The sounds, was also evident in participant descriptions and inevitably led to collective acts of witnessing in this criminal justice context. For example, an investigator describes the initially individual recurrent viewing of video in a violent sexual assault and continues to eventually describe it’s collective viewing; “[I] watched it at the time, and then a few years later once it came to court, and then it resurfacing again, I had to watch it all over again just to refresh my memory and be ready for trial, at which time then I sat down with the jury and watched it with them again” (P- 102). Similarly, other legal professionals also described repeated viewing across extended periods of time, including during the court proceedings. One crown attorney noted “because of the issues in a case, unfortunately you just have to review the video multiple times even in that sort of a public forum...you might view…the video a dozen times with one witness on the stand in a day, going back and forth and back and forth.” (LP-206). While the potential trauma associated with repeated viewing for those employed in the criminal justice system was at times recognized by participants, others focussed on new risks to jurors and the public and wondered to what extent, if any, precautions were possible. For instance, the proliferation of video evidence of violent crime has added new forms of collective was also evident in participant descriptions and inevitably led to collective acts of witnessing in this criminal justice context. For example, an investigator describes the initially individual recurrent viewing of video in a violent sexual assault and continues to eventually describe it’s collective viewing; “[I] watched it at the time, and then a few years later once it came to court, and then it resurfacing again, I had to watch it all over again just to refresh my memory and be ready for trial, at which time then I sat down with the jury and watched it with them again” (P- 102). Similarly, other legal professionals also described repeated viewing across extended periods of time, including during the court proceedings. One crown attorney noted “because of the issues in a case, unfortunately you just have to review the video multiple times even in that sort of a public forum...you might view…the video a dozen times with one witness on the stand in a day, going back and forth and back and forth.” (LP-206). While the potential trauma associated with repeated viewing for those employed in the criminal justice system was at times recognized by participants, others focussed on new risks to jurors and the public and wondered to what extent, if any, precautions were possible. For instance, the proliferation of video evidence of violent crime has added new forms of collective Video Playback, Affective Witnessing, and the Mobility of Trauma the voices, are what stayed with me the longest. The voices…it was there for you and you could not get rid of it” (IDLP204). In addition to repeated listening and viewing during initial investigations or case preparation – which was often an intimate individual act of witnessing –, protracted reviewing affective witnessing of events for jurors. An assistant crown attorney suggests “for a jury who is not trained in doing that, they come in and see stuff...I do think that people are generally naïve as to what actually happens on the streets of the city. Unless you’re somehow connected to Video Playback, Affective Witnessing, and the Mobility of Trauma the criminal justice system, I don’t think people know how bad things actually can be.” (LP-203). Nevertheless, an attorney suggests, “Where the video was relevant, it has to be played in court no matter what because, it’s relevant evidence and the jury needs to see it in order to make a proper determination of the case.” open.” (IDLPLP-206). (LP-201). Further, a judge acknowledges “the general proposition is we do things in public. I’m sorry that that causes distress, but it’s inherent in our system.” (LP-208). Further, the investment in technological advances designed to magnify public engagement in an open court system extends collective affective witnessing beyond the courtroom and into the realm of the public. A judge states “almost never is anything shown in a trial to a jury that isn’t seen by the public…a lot of our courtrooms…have huge screens mounted on the walls just so members of the public can watch the same video that all the jurors and counsel and judge are watching on their individual monitors. And that’s all because they’re just as much entitled to see it as we are. As I say, there is that clash between the openness principle and trying to protect people from seeing things that are highly disturbing.” (LP-208). A prosecuting attorney concurs and adds “[I]t’s played in an open courtroom…and I’ve never had a case where I’ve had a courtroom that’s sealed, so in my experience the courtroom is always Participants’ descriptions of the collective affective witnessing that is inherent to open justice processes exemplified the relational aspects of traumatization. For example, an investigator describes “the jury also watched the numerous hours of video while I sat on the stand quietly just taking in their reaction too. That also stayed with me. It had a lingering effect, I think, more so than me actually watching the video itself, only because they’re not Video Playback, Affective Witnessing, and the Mobility of Trauma privy, or they’re not exposed to these sorts of things. And I just really felt for the whole jury sitting there also having to watch this for the court process. Yeah, that bit was challenging” (P102). This type of trauma “meta-witnessing” by the investigator and others who had previously viewed the video becomes a “self-reflexive practice of witnessing the affects involved in witnessing itself” (Schankweiler, 2020, p. 254), where the reaction of naïve jurors intensifies the impact of the original recording, in an affective feedback loop including the mediated event and courtroom actors. Uncritical and extraneous affective witnessing with colleagues Criminal justice professionals shared a number of workplace circumstances that worked to motivate the sharing of potentially traumatic video evidence outside the general bounds of investigative and legal practices. Whether out of curiosity, shock, or support, participants spoke of indiscriminate viewing and sharing of violent videos as a common occurrence, often not without consequences. A supervising officer suggests a potentially harmful “morbid curiosity” within the profession of policing. “I think when presented with an opportunity to look at something that could be very bothersome or troublesome, I think 99% of police officers are going to have a look, even if they don’t need to…assault, aggravated assault, stabbings, sometimes shootings. Is anybody going to say, oh, I think it’s better I don’t look at that? No. Everybody’s looking.” (P-107). Another investigator suggests a similar curiosity with potentially harmful effects; “people are curious sometimes. Sometimes they catch wind of a case or something that happened in a certain division, and they have no association to it…And they’ll go into the case and take a look at it, or try to view the video which is, of course, oftentimes the most impactful thing to look at.” (P-102). Video Playback, Affective Witnessing, and the Mobility of Trauma At times the motivation to share and seek out videos was to “assist” with investigations for which other officers or legal professionals were responsible. A senior officer asserts “I can tell you people watch stuff all the time. Share it, watch it...Can I see that?...You will see a group of 15 people sitting around a desk watching it...And they don’t do it to watch gore...It’s different for us...It’s to inform…They want to consume it to see if they can assist the investigation” (P-101). This same senior officer then states “There is no governance on who sees what” (P-101). A supervising officer also notes the unrestricted travel of images across investigative units. For example, “there was this kid, a teenager, that was…attacked brutally here in the city. And it was being investigated by the [investigative unit] that worked right next to us. And so, we saw the videos…it might just be whoever happens to be in the office, who hears it playing. It’s like, oh, I heard you’ve got a video, let me see if I can recognise anybody” (P-107). An attorney similarly suggests “we rely on each other for colleagues’ opinions. I am a fairly junior Crown…so I frequently am having people view exactly what I’m viewing to try and assist me in assessing a case or assessing a particular issue...So you have colleagues there to assist you when you’re looking at something that might be disturbing” (LP-206). Notably, at times participants also reported being encouraged to view particularly difficult video evidence with colleagues as a means of mitigating or diffusing the potentially traumatic effects. For example, a legal professional describes “this is more to do with the child pornography element...There was talk within our team, watch it with someone else if you can…It was pretty much, look at it as little as possible, watching it with someone else can make it easier, and don’t eat while you’re doing it” (LP-205). where people say, hey, come over, you’ve got to see this, it’s really bad…And it may be that the person calls you over to view it because they’re so taken aback by it that they maybe just want to see somebody else’s reaction to it” (P-104). An assistant crown attorney similarly suggests the desire to share the experience of shock that occurs with some novel videos. “There are just times where we see things that are just pretty shocking, I guess, and generally speaking, when you see something, you usually call over a colleague where it’s really something that you haven’t seen before” (LP-206). Such “meta-witnessing” can again be understood as a self-reflexive practice of observing the affective reactions of colleagues, thereby potentially intensifying the traumatic impact of the video and expanding trauma’s contagion. These forms of collective affective witnessing came with consequences for professionals. For example, an attorney explains “when somebody tells you about a case, you visualize it or you watch the video, so you take it on...It’s definitely vicarious from talking to your colleagues...now a fatigue has set in where I’ve become very apprehensive about viewing anything unnecessarily…for the most part, I won’t look at their videos, just because I have no interest and I just don’t want to be exposed to it anymore” (LP-205). Here, uncritical and where people say, hey, come over, you’ve got to see this, it’s really bad…And it may be that the person calls you over to view it because they’re so taken aback by it that they maybe just want to see somebody else’s reaction to it” (P-104). An assistant crown attorney similarly suggests the desire to share the experience of shock that occurs with some novel videos. “There are just times where we see things that are just pretty shocking, I guess, and generally speaking, when you see something, you usually call over a colleague where it’s really something that you haven’t seen before” (LP-206). Such “meta-witnessing” can again be understood as a self-reflexive practice of observing the affective reactions of colleagues, thereby potentially intensifying the traumatic impact of the video and expanding trauma’s contagion. These forms of collective affective witnessing came with consequences for professionals. For example, an attorney explains “when somebody tells you about a case, you visualize it or you watch the video, so you take it on...It’s definitely vicarious from talking to your colleagues...now a fatigue has set in where I’ve become very apprehensive about viewing anything unnecessarily…for the most part, I won’t look at their videos, just because I have no interest and I just don’t want to be exposed to it anymore” (LP-205). Here, uncritical and Video Playback, Affective Witnessing, and the Mobility of Trauma Others demonstrated a motivation for seeing the reactions of unsuspecting viewers in their seeking out collective affective witnessing episodes. A senior officer explains “sometimes there’s a thought that you want to show people that video. I know that I’ve had the experience unnecessary exposures can be seen as “compounding and binding together sometimes unrelated life traumas” (Coddington, 2017, p. 66), where multiple affective episodes are interpolated in the bodies of workers (Wetherell, 2015). Affective witnessing of the retraumatization of victims of the events, and impending case resolutions, but were always episodes of collective witnessing. In many cases, however, playing the video evidence provided new information for victims and witnesses, creating new potentially traumatic experiences and memories. For example, a senior officer explains “we have the women [who] are intoxicated at the time of the sexual assault, have no memory of the sexual assault because they’ve ingested something else, a narcotic, and then we have to actually play it for them and have them adopt that that in fact is them. So, those are really hard things.” (P-101). This same officer then provides a specific example and the difficulty with this decision during the investigative process. “We have a victim, we found her sexual assault on his phone. She has no memory of it. So, the ethical dilemma is when do you show this to them?” An attorney similarly suggests “I’ve had people who have been really terribly assaulted and it’s captured on video…I think for both witnesses and victims, there are always things on the video that they didn’t notice or they didn’t remember until they see the video. So, it can be triggering and it may very well be negative.” (LP-206). This attorney also suggests “I can imagine that for complainants and witnesses who have to constantly be re-exposed to violent video evidence while they’re of the events, and impending case resolutions, but were always episodes of collective witnessing. In many cases, however, playing the video evidence provided new information for victims and witnesses, creating new potentially traumatic experiences and memories. For example, a senior officer explains “we have the women [who] are intoxicated at the time of the sexual assault, have no memory of the sexual assault because they’ve ingested something else, a narcotic, and then we have to actually play it for them and have them adopt that that in fact is them. So, those are really hard things.” (P-101). This same officer then provides a specific example and the difficulty with this decision during the investigative process. “We have a victim, we found her sexual assault on his phone. She has no memory of it. So, the ethical dilemma is when do you show this to them?” An attorney similarly suggests “I’ve had people who have been really terribly assaulted and it’s captured on video…I think for both witnesses and victims, there are always things on the video that they didn’t notice or they didn’t remember until they see the video. So, it can be triggering and it may very well be negative.” (LP-206). This attorney also suggests “I can imagine that for complainants and witnesses who have to constantly be re-exposed to violent video evidence while they’re Video Playback, Affective Witnessing, and the Mobility of Trauma For various reasons, victims, witnesses, and family members may be presented with the video evidence of the victimization in question at any point throughout the criminal justice process. Investigative and legal decisions to show victims were sometimes predicated on video ownership, identification practices, ethical considerations related to the victims’ age or memory testifying, I know that that can be very retraumatizing. Even if they only have to view it once when you’re preparing them for trial and then it winds up being a guilty plea, it’s still very traumatizing” (LP-206). usually stay with them, with the officer, and we’re usually in a room sometimes with a victim witness worker…So, in those contexts we sit, we all watch the video, and sometimes I’ll have to play it multiple times to be able to identify areas where a witness is going to be expected to have questions asked of them” (LP-206). This meta-witnessing with those involved in the original trauma further reverberates the suffering captured on video as victims (re)experience events. For example, an assistant crown attorney recalls of these moments and cases as “the ones that stayed” (LP-203). She recalls, “I have the videos of...the sexual violence and then I have the real witness that I have to show that to and have her describe what happened…Those kinds of cases are the ones that stayed” and then adds “for the homicide type cases, those videos were always hard because I knew I had to show the family…no one prepares you to sit down and have to show a family their child or the family member on a video either being killed or have their body there…It’s just having to deal with a live witness or a family and then having to explain that to them and deal with that with them.” This attorney speaks extensively about the idea that viewing video evidence of violent crimes with victims and witnesses is central to the traumatic impact that cases have. “[T]he very first homicide I usually stay with them, with the officer, and we’re usually in a room sometimes with a victim witness worker…So, in those contexts we sit, we all watch the video, and sometimes I’ll have to play it multiple times to be able to identify areas where a witness is going to be expected to have questions asked of them” (LP-206). This meta-witnessing with those involved in the original trauma further reverberates the suffering captured on video as victims (re)experience events. For example, an assistant crown attorney recalls of these moments and cases as “the ones that stayed” (LP-203). She recalls, “I have the videos of...the sexual violence and then I have the real witness that I have to show that to and have her describe what happened…Those kinds of cases are the ones that stayed” and then adds “for the homicide type cases, those videos were always hard because I knew I had to show the family…no one prepares you to sit down and have to show a family their child or the family member on a video either being killed or have their body there…It’s just having to deal with a live witness or a family and then having to explain that to them and deal with that with them.” This attorney speaks extensively about the idea that viewing video evidence of violent crimes with victims and witnesses is central to the traumatic impact that cases have. “[T]he very first homicide I Video Playback, Affective Witnessing, and the Mobility of Trauma An assistant crown attorney explicitly reminds us of the collective affective witnessing that occurs during this process and the eventual embodied trauma. “When I show those videos to a witness…it’s usually myself, the witness and a police officer…I try to prepare people for what it is that they’re about to see…just telling them that it’s a video that captures…Then I did…I had the responsibility of showing family the videos of her body in the apartment…I have another case…where he filmed every sexual act with his victim and those sorts of things sometimes rear their ugly head in your memory…And I think a part of that was because I was Video Playback, Affective Witnessing, and the Mobility of Trauma the one that had to show them the videos with the officer” (LP-203). From this attorney, we can see that the experience of trauma and its mobility are profoundly relational, very much a process of the shared affecting episode and the collective witnessing and re-witnessing with the survivors of the original traumatic event. Uncontrolled mobility of trauma and affective witnessing in the broader public Not all video evidence of violent crime is originally created with private devices for private storage. Videos may immediately be shared on social media, other online platforms, from person to person, the media may gain access to video at any point, and others yet are produced precisely for mass distribution and collective public consumption. The occurrence or possibility of wider dissemination was of concern for those working directly with horrifically violent material. An analyst in national security well versed in extremist propaganda, suggested it wasn’t only the improved production quality of video that was concerning for its traumatic impact but also the “ability to make it accessible to people in a variety of countries in a variety of languages almost instantaneously” (P-105). Another senior officer in national security also noted the significance of video and its mobility; “his video was a really, really significant piece…he intended to go out and thought he was going to blow himself up and die, and this video would then be broadcast around the world for all the supporters” (P-103). Cases involving graphic video evidence draw further public and media attention to investigations and court proceedings. A senior officer suggests, “we’re very careful with what we show to the public…It’s to either have the public assist us in our case or to inform them…We’re not looking for secondary trauma, vicarious trauma. We talk about ethical decision-making all the time when it comes to what we release” (P-101). This same officer then Video Playback, Affective Witnessing, and the Mobility of Trauma recalls a case involving hours of video concerning a violent sexual assault and efforts to limit access; “I asked to see a portion of it so I could articulate to the Crown attorney what…we needed…But other than that, nobody else was allowed to look at it, until of course the newspapers were able to gain access. The court allowed them to watch it. We had actually lobbied not to have them watch it.” (P-101). A legal professional similarly recalls of a particularly heinous case involving rape, torture and murder, that the media “lobbied heavily for access to the videotape” and then adds “I don’t know if that was necessary, to be honest...I feel like a written transcript would have been sufficient.” (LP-202). Another legal professional voices dismay for a victim whose image was shared by the media. “I don’t even know which media outlet it was that released this…before the trial even started it was out there…ultimately, very upsetting for this young girl” (LP-204). A judge suggests, however, “nowadays we rely to, I think, a larger extent on the discretion of the media that even if they want a copy of something, they won’t generally publish it because they also recognize that it’s not the thing to do…So, I think if there’s a graphic video of a murder for example, even if the media were to get a copy of it, I don’t think you would ever turn on the 6:00 news and see it played” (LP-208). Criminal justice professionals often voiced distress and their inherited sense of responsibility to act on the suffering of victims when discussing victims, survivors, and witnesses involved with the images, both throughout and beyond investigations and court proceedings. A legal professional explains “I was completely empathetic to families in that case who didn’t want a possibility that those videotapes would get in the hands of the media…they were worried that somehow it would get out.” (LP-202). A senior officer recalls “what she was Video Playback, Affective Witnessing, and the Mobility of Trauma really worried about was that video was still going to be out there…she can never get that video off the internet. And that’s the struggle for us. And it’s difficult because you do want to protect her and see that this gets closure” (P-104). A legal professional involved with creating “laws to try to avoid that, to make sure that the people that disseminated that kind of information were not outside the boundaries of the law”, believed “that’s an evolving area that has to really be aware of the impact of those videotapes on the person who is videotaped.” (LP-202). Notably, media access and broader public dissemination of videos in general was challenging for participants – also limiting their ability to disconnect. A senior officer believes “when it was just part of our world, it was probably more manageable. But now it’s part of everybody’s world…now when I leave work, it’s there again…It’s on TV. It’s on the internet. It’s on the web. It’s everywhere.” (P-101). An investigator, singularly privy to the video evidence in a sexual assault investigation similarly describes how media access to the graphic video evidence during the trial came as an additional source of distress, because of its public release. “[O]nce the media who were also able to view some portions of it, or all of it…when they reported on some of the instances or some of the scenes, it was kind of strange because for so long, I’m pretty sure I was the only one that watched it from start to finish…So, it was almost like just something that I knew about…and then once the media started putting up portions of it…It was almost like other people were let in on the horrors of it…it was just so graphic.” (P 102). Here, the painful and embodied knowledge acquired through individually witnessing, analyzing, and becoming responsible to another’s trauma is at first experienced as an intimate and private affecting episode by the investigator, but then becomes a “profoundly reliable, those who work with it are now engaging with evidence of violent crime in novel ways. Findings from this qualitative examination suggest new forms of affective witnessing and an unprecedented emotional proximity to the traumatic experiences of others. In attending to the affective-discursive moments in interviews with criminal justice professionals as they described their encounters with video evidence of violent crime, we have identified several key categories that both serve to outline the varying circumstances of their encounters with video evidence, as well as the ways in which the mobility of trauma is supported through relational practices: playback in the investigative and pre-trial process; sharing videos among colleagues; playing videos for victims, witnesses, and families; and transmission in the broader public. The transmission of traumas captured on video have provided trauma with new mobility, thereby amplifying affective episodes and intensifying trauma contagion with each playback, whether an individual or collective act of witnessing. While the key categories are explicitly derived from the affective-discursive moments that arose from engaging with violent video evidence in a criminal justice context, we imagine the processes of viewing, analyzing, sharing, and witnessing the reactions of other viewers, are all affecting episodes that may occur in common reliable, those who work with it are now engaging with evidence of violent crime in novel ways. Findings from this qualitative examination suggest new forms of affective witnessing and an unprecedented emotional proximity to the traumatic experiences of others. In attending to the affective-discursive moments in interviews with criminal justice professionals as they described their encounters with video evidence of violent crime, we have identified several key categories that both serve to outline the varying circumstances of their encounters with video evidence, as well as the ways in which the mobility of trauma is supported through relational practices: playback in the investigative and pre-trial process; sharing videos among colleagues; playing videos for victims, witnesses, and families; and transmission in the broader public. The transmission of traumas captured on video have provided trauma with new mobility, thereby amplifying affective episodes and intensifying trauma contagion with each playback, whether an individual or collective act of witnessing. While the key categories are explicitly derived from the affective-discursive moments that arose from engaging with violent video evidence in a criminal justice context, we imagine the processes of viewing, analyzing, sharing, and witnessing the reactions of other viewers, are all affecting episodes that may occur in common Video Playback, Affective Witnessing, and the Mobility of Trauma intersubjective experience” through a painfully public and revealing collective encounter as the event is transmitted and witnessed en masse (Rossmanith, 2015, p. 172). Discussion Given the increasing pervasiveness and dependence on video as highly objective and in other contexts. Human rights abuse workers, social media or online content moderators, and journalists, for example, may practice and experience the witnessing of recorded traumatic embodying events as “second nature”. Whereas “vicarious witnessing” traditionally conceptualizes the witnessing of events as occurring through the imagination – by assembling abstract information such as stories and physical artifacts to create a representation of the event (Keats, 2005) – we found video evidence of violent crime may prevent viewers from experiencing safer, more abstract semblances of past events. These videos do represent real events and possess “the unique tangibility of a real moment captured in material form” (Biber, 2013, p. 1040). Accordingly, rather than “imagining what it would be like to be in their shoes” (Coddington, 2017, p. 68), video evidence of violent crime immortalizes actual events and amplifies aspects of trauma that might have otherwise been fleeting, imperceptible or unknowable, by “put[ting] you in the shoes of the participants”. The experience of trauma through affective witnessing of video evidence of violent crime is one relational dynamic that keeps witnessing active, thus expanding the mobility of trauma, its reach and potential impacts. Within psychology, the trauma film paradigm provides several lines of evidence of the posttraumatic effects of watching distressing films (Arnaudova & Hagenaars, 2017; Holmes & Bourne, 2008; James et al., 2016) that are relevant for the criminal justice context. For instance, embodying events as “second nature”. Whereas “vicarious witnessing” traditionally conceptualizes the witnessing of events as occurring through the imagination – by assembling abstract information such as stories and physical artifacts to create a representation of the event (Keats, 2005) – we found video evidence of violent crime may prevent viewers from experiencing safer, more abstract semblances of past events. These videos do represent real events and possess “the unique tangibility of a real moment captured in material form” (Biber, 2013, p. 1040). Accordingly, rather than “imagining what it would be like to be in their shoes” (Coddington, 2017, p. 68), video evidence of violent crime immortalizes actual events and amplifies aspects of trauma that might have otherwise been fleeting, imperceptible or unknowable, by “put[ting] you in the shoes of the participants”. The experience of trauma through affective witnessing of video evidence of violent crime is one relational dynamic that keeps witnessing active, thus expanding the mobility of trauma, its reach and potential impacts. Within psychology, the trauma film paradigm provides several lines of evidence of the posttraumatic effects of watching distressing films (Arnaudova & Hagenaars, 2017; Holmes & Bourne, 2008; James et al., 2016) that are relevant for the criminal justice context. For instance, Video Playback, Affective Witnessing, and the Mobility of Trauma events in similar ways (Baker et al., 2020; Breslow, 2018; Pinchevski, 2022; Weidmann & Papsdorf, 2010). The processes of investigative and pre-trial analysis of video reported by participants centred on close examination and repeated viewing to notice and memorize details, eventually repeated exposures to trauma videos (Badawi et al., 2020; James et al., 2015) as well as very close viewing for critical information gathering using local – rather than general global – processing of intricate details (Hagenaars et al., 2016) may both lead to greater posttraumatic 2022; Streb et al., 2017), increasing the risk for secondary trauma when the audio content involves the traumatic experiences of others (Etherington, 2007; Kiyimba & O’Reilly, 2016). For instance, previous research on the stressful aspects of 911 communications (emergency call- taking and dispatch) suggests that voice work in high trauma exposure settings may also lead to the embodiment of distant others’ trauma, even without the presence of moving images (Birze, Paradis, et al., 2022). As has been previously reported by our team, the repeated viewing of video evidence of violent crimes can, not only re-victimize the actual victim(s) depicted in the images (Regehr et al., 2021), but it can also result in profound traumatic responses as the experiences of others are embodied by the viewer(Birze, Regehr, et al., 2022b). The inclination to share video among colleagues not directly involved in cases was also apparent. Whereas talking about and sharing experiences of vicarious affective witnessing may lead to a diffusion or dispersion of posttraumatic stress responses among groups (Keats, 2005), regular exposure to videos from colleagues was at times experienced as distressing. Accordingly, in some situations there was only so much trauma dispersion that could occur before teams and individuals could no longer “take it on”. Indeed, and in line with the trauma 2022; Streb et al., 2017), increasing the risk for secondary trauma when the audio content involves the traumatic experiences of others (Etherington, 2007; Kiyimba & O’Reilly, 2016). For instance, previous research on the stressful aspects of 911 communications (emergency call- taking and dispatch) suggests that voice work in high trauma exposure settings may also lead to the embodiment of distant others’ trauma, even without the presence of moving images (Birze, Paradis, et al., 2022). As has been previously reported by our team, the repeated viewing of video evidence of violent crimes can, not only re-victimize the actual victim(s) depicted in the images (Regehr et al., 2021), but it can also result in profound traumatic responses as the experiences of others are embodied by the viewer(Birze, Regehr, et al., 2022b). The inclination to share video among colleagues not directly involved in cases was also apparent. Whereas talking about and sharing experiences of vicarious affective witnessing may lead to a diffusion or dispersion of posttraumatic stress responses among groups (Keats, 2005), regular exposure to videos from colleagues was at times experienced as distressing. Accordingly, in some situations there was only so much trauma dispersion that could occur before teams and individuals could no longer “take it on”. Indeed, and in line with the trauma Video Playback, Affective Witnessing, and the Mobility of Trauma impacts such as higher levels of re-experiencing videoed images, or flashbacks. Furthermore, motion in facial expressions provides more emotional information to viewers, thus activating more emotion-specific brain regions (Trautmann et al., 2009), while the emotional information in audio recordings likely adds another layer of affective activation (Doughty & Drozdzewski, contagion work of Coddington (2017), viewing videos from colleagues’ potentially traumatic cases appeared to compound and bind together the multiple and persistent unrelated traumas, Video Playback, Affective Witnessing, and the Mobility of Trauma thereby spreading and expanding “trauma’s mobility as it travels in and through bodies” (Coddington & Micieli-Voutsinas, 2017, p. 52). The affective witnessing our participants experienced was at times also accompanied by close and real encounters with those who bore witness to the original event and suffering. This additional direct encounter with once-distant sufferers appeared to amplify the magnitude of affective witnessing and its traumatic repercussions, through what might be considered an affective meta-witnessing episode. Having a “real” or “live” witness or naïve jury to review video with, proved to be a profoundly distressing experience where participants both watched the affective episode on video while simultaneously watching victims and witnesses live or “relive” their traumas in real-time. Their co-presence in the direct encounter, alongside their co-presence in a distant place of suffering presented a double bill for participants that then became “the ones that stayed”. Perhaps the sense of “thereness” described by Ellis (2009) as “an almost involuntary response to seeing lifelike moving images with synchronized sound” in part reflects the material changes that occur in the brain as we view emotionally charged audiovisual images or affecting episodes. The neurophysiology of vicarious stress and trauma offers insight into how the physiological resonance or contagion of trauma in each of the above circumstances might occur, at least in part (Dimitroff et al., 2017; Engert et al., 2019; White & Buchanan, 2016). Visual and auditory representations of another’s experiences may be transmitted between individuals and reproduced in witnesses via the mirror neuron system; neurons that discharge during both first-person execution of an action and when observing that action executed by another, leading to an embodied simulation of another’s experience (Engert et al., 2019; Video Playback, Affective Witnessing, and the Mobility of Trauma Iacoboni, 2009). In other words, “we might empathically share the states of others because seeing their states triggers representations of corresponding states in our brain” (Keysers & Gazzola, 2009, p. 666). Thus, through the triggering of the mirror neuron system across multiple brain regions, when immersed in others’ traumatic experiences, observers are suggested to “explicitly share the target’s stress response”, including the emotional bodily states of victims captured on video (Engert et al., 2019, p. 139; Keysers & Gazzola, 2009). This mechanism of trauma contagion has been proposed for mental health clinicians experiencing vicarious traumatization and secondary traumatic stress as a result of their empathically engaged work with victims of trauma (Isobel & Angus-Leppan, 2018; Rasmussen & Bliss, 2014). Conclusion The “perpetual visibility” and audibility of these real moments are what allow trauma to travel in, through, and between bodies, across space and time with alarming ease (Biber, 2017, p. 19). Indeed, the rapid pace of technological advance and exponential production and transmissibility of video has far outpaced organizational awareness, preparedness and response (Dodge et al., 2019; Kimpel, 2021), suggesting the need for improved understanding of safety measures (Birze, Regehr, et al., 2022a; Regehr et al., 2022). Furthermore, while the criminal justice system has no control over the sharing of videos released under freedom of information applications and other forms of public transmission, an emerging jurisprudence of sensitivity (Biber, 2013; Birze, Regehr, et al., 2022b; Regehr et al., 2022) appeals to criminal justice actors to consider the mobility of trauma in a way that recognizes both the short and long-term impact of every playback for every viewer. As Katherine Biber has described in her analysis of Video Playback, Affective Witnessing, and the Mobility of Trauma technological advances in visual evidence in the context of justice, “[t]his new visibility, the ability to see [and hear] secrets, demands a sensitive response” (Biber, 2015, p. 237). Video Playback, Affective Witnessing, and the Mobility of Trauma References Arnaudova, I., & Hagenaars, M. A. (2017). Lights… action: Comparison of trauma films for use in the trauma film paradigm. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 93, 67-77. Badawi, A., Berle, D., Rogers, K., & Steel, Z. (2020). Do cognitive tasks reduce intrusive-memory frequency after exposure to analogue trauma? An experimental replication. Clinical Psychological Science, 8(3), 569-583. Baker, E., Stover, E., Haar, R., Lampros, A., & Koenig, A. (2020). Safer Viewing: A Study of Secondary Trauma Mitigation Techniques in Open Source Investigations. Health and Human Rights, 22(1), 293. Bens, J. (2020). Affective Witnessing in the Courtroom. Parallax, 26(3), 271-285. Biber, K. (2013). In Crime’s Archive: The Cultural Afterlife of Criminal Evidence. The British journal of criminology, 53, 1033-1049. Biber, K. (2015). Open secrets, open justice. In Secrecy, Law and Society (pp. 234-250). Routledge. Biber, K. (2017). Law, evidence and representation. In M. Brown & E. Carrabine (Eds.), Routledge International Handbook of Visual Criminology (pp. 13-22). Routledge. Birks, M., & Mills, J. (2015). Grounded theory: A practical guide. Sage. Birze, A., Paradis, E., LeBlanc, V., Regehr, C., & Einstein, G. (2022). Gender in the Flesh: Emotional Labour, Gender Conformity, and Allostatic Load in Police Communicators. Work, employment and society. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1177/09500170221080388 Birze, A., Regehr, C., & Regehr, K. (2022a). Organizational support for the potentially traumatic impact of video evidence of violent crime in the criminal justice system: ‘We’re almost making more victims’. International Review of Victimology, OnlineFirst. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1177/02697580221112436 Birze, A., Regehr, K., & Regehr, C. (2022b). Workplace trauma in a digital age: the impact of video evidence of violent crime on criminal justice professionals. Journal of interpersonal violence, 08862605221090571. Braun, V., & Clarke, V. (2021). To saturate or not to saturate? Questioning data saturation as a useful concept for thematic analysis and sample-size rationales. Qualitative research in sport, exercise and health, 13(2), 201-216. Breslow, J. (2018). Moderating the ‘worst of humanity’: sexuality, witnessing, and the digital life of coloniality. Porn Studies, 5(3), 225-240. Brookman, F., & Jones, H. (2021). Capturing killers: the construction of CCTV evidence during homicide investigations. Policing and Society, 1-20. Charmaz, K. (2000). Grounded theory: Objectivist and Constructivist methods. In N. Denzin & Y. Lincoln (Eds.), Handbook of Qualitative Research (pp. 523-544). Sage. Charmaz, K. (2008). Grounded theory as an emergent method. Handbook of emergent methods, 155, 172. Charmaz, K. (2014). Constructing Grounded Theory (2nd ed.). Sage. Charmaz, K., & Thornberg, R. (2021). The pursuit of quality in grounded theory. Qualitative Research in Psychology, 18(3), 305-327. Video Playback, Affective Witnessing, and the Mobility of Trauma Coddington, K. (2017). Contagious trauma: Reframing the spatial mobility of trauma within advocacy work. Emotion, Space and Society, 24, 66-73. Coddington, K., & Micieli-Voutsinas, J. (2017). On trauma, geography, and mobility: towards geographies of trauma. Emotion, space and society., 24, 52-56. Cook, P. J., Berglund, A., & Triano, M. (2021). More and better video evidence for police investigations of shootings: Chicago's area technology centers. Policing: An International Journal. Corbin, J., & Strauss, A. (2008). Basics of qualitative research: techniques and procedures for developing grounded theory. Sage Publications Inc. Cumiskey, K. M., & Hjorth, L. (2019). “I wish they could have answered their phones”: Mobile communication in mass shootings. Death studies, 43(7), 414-425. Dimitroff, S. J., Kardan, O., Necka, E. A., Decety, J., Berman, M. G., & Norman, G. J. (2017, 2017/07/21). Physiological dynamics of stress contagion. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 6168. Dodge, A., Spencer, D., Ricciardelli, R., & Ballucci, D. (2019). “This isn’t your father’s police force”: Digital evidence in sexual assault investigations. Australian & New Zealand Journal of Criminology, 52(4), 499-515. Doughty, K., & Drozdzewski, D. (2022). Sonic methods, sonic affects. Emotion, Space and Society, 42. Drozdzewski, D., & Dominey-Howes, D. (2015). Research and trauma: Understanding the impact of traumatic content and places on the researcher. Emotion, Space and Society, 17, 17- 21. Ellis, J. (2009). What are we expected to feel? Witness, textuality and the audiovisual. Screen, 50(1), 67-76. Engert, V., Linz, R., & Grant, J. A. (2019). Embodied stress: The physiological resonance of psychosocial stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 105, 138-146. Video Playback, Affective Witnessing, and the Mobility of Trauma Coddington, K. (2017). Contagious trauma: Reframing the spatial mobility of trauma within advocacy work. Emotion, Space and Society, 24, 66-73. Coddington, K., & Micieli-Voutsinas, J. (2017). On trauma, geography, and mobility: towards geographies of trauma. Emotion, space and society., 24, 52-56. Cook, P. J., Berglund, A., & Triano, M. (2021). More and better video evidence for police investigations of shootings: Chicago's area technology centers. Policing: An International Journal. Corbin, J., & Strauss, A. (2008). Basics of qualitative research: techniques and procedures for developing grounded theory. Sage Publications Inc. Cumiskey, K. M., & Hjorth, L. (2019). “I wish they could have answered their phones”: Mobile communication in mass shootings. Death studies, 43(7), 414-425. Dimitroff, S. J., Kardan, O., Necka, E. A., Decety, J., Berman, M. G., & Norman, G. J. (2017, 2017/07/21). Physiological dynamics of stress contagion. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 6168. Dodge, A., Spencer, D., Ricciardelli, R., & Ballucci, D. (2019). “This isn’t your father’s police force”: Digital evidence in sexual assault investigations. Australian & New Zealand Journal of Criminology, 52(4), 499-515. Doughty, K., & Drozdzewski, D. (2022). Sonic methods, sonic affects. Emotion, Space and Society, 42. Drozdzewski, D., & Dominey-Howes, D. (2015). Research and trauma: Understanding the impact of traumatic content and places on the researcher. Emotion, Space and Society, 17, 17- 21. Ellis, J. (2009). What are we expected to feel? Witness, textuality and the audiovisual. Screen, 50(1), 67-76. Engert, V., Linz, R., & Grant, J. A. (2019). Embodied stress: The physiological resonance of psychosocial stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 105, 138-146. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2018.12.221 Etherington, K. (2007). Working with traumatic stories: From transcriber to witness. International Journal of Social Research Methodology, 10(2), 85-97. Glaser, B., & Strauss, A. (1967). The Discovery of Grounded Theory: Strategies for Qualitative Research. Aldine de Gruyter. Hagenaars, M. A., Engelhard, I. M., & Putman, P. (2016). Eye for Detail: Local versus Global Visual Processing Style Predicts the Development of Re-experiencing after Analogue Trauma. Journal of Experimental Psychopathology, 7(3), 391-403. Hemsworth, K. (2016). ‘Feeling the range’: Emotional geographies of sound in prisons. Emotion, Space and Society, 20, 90-97. Hjorth, L., & Cumiskey, K. M. (2018). Mobiles facing death: Affective witnessing and the intimate companionship of devices. Cultural Studies Review, 24(2), 166-180. Holmes, E. A., & Bourne, C. (2008). Inducing and modulating intrusive emotional memories: A review of the trauma film paradigm. Acta psychologica, 127(3), 553-566. Iacoboni, M. (2009). Imitation, empathy, and mirror neurons. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 653-670. Isobel, S., & Angus-Leppan, G. (2018). Neuro-reciprocity and vicarious trauma in psychiatrists. Australasian Psychiatry, 26(4), 388-390. Kimpel, A. (2021). Violent Videos: Criminal Defense in a Digital Age. Georgia State University Law Review, 37(2), 1-68. Kiyimba, N., & O’Reilly, M. (2016). The risk of secondary traumatic stress in the qualitative transcription process: A research note. Qualitative Research, 16(4), 468-476. Kyriakidou, M. (2015). Media witnessing: Exploring the audience of distant suffering. Media, Culture & Society, 37(2), 215-231. Lietz, C. A., & Zayas, L. E. (2010). Evaluating qualitative research for social work practitioners. Advances in Social Work, 11(2), 188-202. Lindegaard, M. R., & Bernasco, W. (2018). Lessons learned from crime caught on camera. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 55(1), 155-186. Low, J. (2019). A pragmatic definition of the concept of theoretical saturation. Sociological Focus, 52(2), 131-139. McCracken, G. (1988). The Long Interview. Sage Publications, Inc. McGrane, C., Hjorth, L., & Akama, Y. (2021). Careful attunements: the choreographing of care and affective witnessing through media practices during, and after, crisis. Media, Culture & Society, 01634437211040952. Peters, J. D. (2001). Witnessing. Media, Culture & Society, 23, 707-723. Philpot, R., Liebst, L. S., Møller, K. K., Lindegaard, M. R., & Levine, M. (2019). Capturing violence in the night-time economy: A review of established and emerging methodologies. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 46, 56-65. Pinchevski, A. (2022). Social media’s canaries: content moderators between digital labor and mediated trauma. Media, Culture & Society, 01634437221122226. Rasmussen, B., & Bliss, S. (2014). Beneath the surface: An exploration of neurobiological alterations in therapists working with trauma. Smith College Studies in Social Work, 84(2-3), 332-349. Regehr, C., Regehr, K., & Birze, A. (2022). Traumatic residue, mediated remembering and video evidence of sexual violence: A case study. International Journal of Law and Psychiatry, 81, 101778. Kimpel, A. (2021). Violent Videos: Criminal Defense in a Digital Age. Georgia State University Law Review, 37(2), 1-68. Kiyimba, N., & O’Reilly, M. (2016). The risk of secondary traumatic stress in the qualitative transcription process: A research note. Qualitative Research, 16(4), 468-476. Kyriakidou, M. (2015). Media witnessing: Exploring the audience of distant suffering. Media, Culture & Society, 37(2), 215-231. Lietz, C. A., & Zayas, L. E. (2010). Evaluating qualitative research for social work practitioners. Advances in Social Work, 11(2), 188-202. Lindegaard, M. R., & Bernasco, W. (2018). Lessons learned from crime caught on camera. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 55(1), 155-186. Low, J. (2019). A pragmatic definition of the concept of theoretical saturation. Sociological Focus, 52(2), 131-139. McCracken, G. (1988). The Long Interview. Sage Publications, Inc. McGrane, C., Hjorth, L., & Akama, Y. (2021). Careful attunements: the choreographing of care and affective witnessing through media practices during, and after, crisis. Media, Culture & Society, 01634437211040952. Peters, J. D. (2001). Witnessing. Media, Culture & Society, 23, 707-723. Philpot, R., Liebst, L. S., Møller, K. K., Lindegaard, M. R., & Levine, M. (2019). Capturing violence in the night-time economy: A review of established and emerging methodologies. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 46, 56-65. Pinchevski, A. (2022). Social media’s canaries: content moderators between digital labor and mediated trauma. Media, Culture & Society, 01634437221122226. Rasmussen, B., & Bliss, S. (2014). Beneath the surface: An exploration of neurobiological alterations in therapists working with trauma. Smith College Studies in Social Work, 84(2-3), 332-349. Regehr, C., Regehr, K., & Birze, A. (2022). Traumatic residue, mediated remembering and video evidence of sexual violence: A case study. International Journal of Law and Psychiatry, 81, 101778. Video Playback, Affective Witnessing, and the Mobility of Trauma James, E. L., Bonsall, M. B., Hoppitt, L., Tunbridge, E. M., Geddes, J. R., Milton, A. L., & Holmes, E. A. (2015). Computer game play reduces intrusive memories of experimental trauma via reconsolidation-update mechanisms. Psychological science, 26(8), 1201-1215. James, E. L., Lau-Zhu, A., Clark, I. A., Visser, R. M., Hagenaars, M. A., & Holmes, E. A. (2016). The trauma film paradigm as an experimental psychopathology model of psychological trauma: Intrusive memories and beyond. Clinical Psychology Review, 47, 106-142. Keats, P. A. (2005). Vicarious witnessing in European concentration camps: Imagining the trauma of another. Traumatology, 11(3), 171-187. Keysers, C., & Gazzola, V. (2009). Expanding the mirror: vicarious activity for actions, emotions, and sensations. Current opinion in neurobiology, 19(6), 666-671. Regehr, K., Birze, A., & Regehr, C. (2021). Technology facilitated re-victimization: How video evidence of sexual violence contributes to mediated cycles of abuse. Crime, Media, Culture. Regehr, K., Regehr, C., & Glancy, G. (2019). Murder at the dinner table: family narratives of forensic mental health professionals. Journal of Loss and Trauma, 24(1), 31-49. How durable are they over time and does memory integration reduce them? Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 57, 88-95. Trautmann, S. A., Fehr, T., & Herrmann, M. (2009). Emotions in motion: dynamic compared to static facial expressions of disgust and happiness reveal more widespread emotion- specific activations. Brain research, 1284, 100-115. Weidmann, A., & Papsdorf, J. (2010). Witnessing trauma in the newsroom: Posttraumatic symptoms in television journalists exposed to violent news clips. The Journal of nervous and mental disease, 198(4), 264-271. Westera, N. J., & Powell, M. B. (2017). Prosecutors’ perceptions of how to improve the quality of evidence in domestic violence cases. Policing and Society, 27(2), 157-172. Wetherell, M. (2012). Affect and emotion: A new social science understanding. Sage Publications. Wetherell, M. (2013). Affect and discourse–What’s the problem? From affect as excess to affective/discursive practice. Subjectivity, 6(4), 349-368. Wetherell, M. (2015). Trends in the turn to affect: A social psychological critique. Body & Society, 21(2), 139-166. White, C. N., & Buchanan, T. W. (2016). Empathy for the stressed. Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology, 2(4), 311-324. How durable are they over time and does memory integration reduce them? Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 57, 88-95. Trautmann, S. A., Fehr, T., & Herrmann, M. (2009). Emotions in motion: dynamic compared to static facial expressions of disgust and happiness reveal more widespread emotion- specific activations. Brain research, 1284, 100-115. Weidmann, A., & Papsdorf, J. (2010). Witnessing trauma in the newsroom: Posttraumatic symptoms in television journalists exposed to violent news clips. The Journal of nervous and mental disease, 198(4), 264-271. Westera, N. J., & Powell, M. B. (2017). Prosecutors’ perceptions of how to improve the quality of evidence in domestic violence cases. Policing and Society, 27(2), 157-172. Wetherell, M. (2012). Affect and emotion: A new social science understanding. Sage Publications. Wetherell, M. (2013). Affect and discourse–What’s the problem? From affect as excess to affective/discursive practice. Subjectivity, 6(4), 349-368. Wetherell, M. (2015). Trends in the turn to affect: A social psychological critique. Body & Society, 21(2), 139-166. White, C. N., & Buchanan, T. W. (2016). Empathy for the stressed. Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology, 2(4), 311-324. Video Playback, Affective Witnessing, and the Mobility of Trauma Richardson, M., & Schankweiler, K. (2020). Introduction: Affective Witnessing as Theory and Practice. Parallax, 26(3), 235-253. Rossmanith, K. (2015). Affect and the judicial assessment of offenders: Feeling and judging remorse. Body & Society, 21(2), 167-193. Schankweiler, K. (2020). Reaction Images and Metawitnessing. Parallax, 26(3), 254-270. Strauss, A., & Corbin, J. (1998). Basics of qualitative research techniques. Sage publications Thousand Oaks, CA. Streb, M., Conway, M. A., & Michael, T. (2017). Conditioned responses to trauma reminders: